Simon Aronson Simply Simon Pdf Viewer

Simon Aronson Simply Simon Pdf Reader. 7/16/2017 0 Comments Etimologia del termine. La parola italiana libro deriva dal latino liber. Il vocabolo originariamente significava anche 'corteccia', ma visto che era un materiale. Tuesdays with Morrie: An Old Man, a Young Man, and Life's Greatest Lesson. Simon and Virginia Aronson have been reading minds as a team for 40 years. And magic simply demonstrates that they’re wrong—or, at least, creates the illusion of it.”. The University of Chicago Magazine (ISSN-0041-9508) is published quarterly by the University of Chicago in cooperation with the Alumni Association.

  1. Simply Simon Thoroughbred Gelding
  2. Simply Simon Pizza
  3. Simply Simon Malls
  4. Simon Aronson Simply Simon Pdf Viewer Online
During the Bosnian War (1992–95), Serbian Večernje novosti published a war-report supposedly from Bosnia titled 'Болно подсећанје' ('Painful reminder') and illustrated with a well-known Uroš Predić's painting from 1888 (right), presented as an actual photograph from the scene (left) of, as stated in report below image, a 'Serbian boy whose whole family was killed by Bosnian Muslims'. Predić's original painting is titled 'Siroče na majčinom grobu' (Orphan at mother's grave).[1]

Propaganda is information that is not objective and is used primarily to influence an audience and further an agenda, often by presenting facts selectively to encourage a particular synthesis or perception, or using loaded language to produce an emotional rather than a rational response to the information that is presented.[2] Propaganda is often associated with material prepared by governments, but activist groups, companies, religious organizations and the media can also produce propaganda.

In the 20th century, the term propaganda had often been associated with a manipulative approach, but propaganda historically was a neutral descriptive term.[2][3]

A wide range of materials and media are used for conveying propaganda messages, which changed as new technologies were invented, including paintings, cartoons, posters, pamphlets, films, radio shows, TV shows, and websites. More recently, the digital age has given rise to new ways of disseminating propaganda, for example, through the use of bots and algorithms to create computational propaganda and spread fake or biased news using social media.

In a 1929 literary debate with Edward Bernays, Everett Dean Martin argues that, 'Propaganda is making puppets of us. We are moved by hidden strings which the propagandist manipulates.'[4][5]

  • 4Types
  • 6Models
  • 7Children
  • 9References
  • 10Further reading

Etymology[edit]

Propaganda is a modern Latin word, the gerundive form of propagare, meaning to spread or to propagate, thus propaganda means that which is to be propagated.[6] Originally this word derived from a new administrative body of the Catholic church (congregation) created in 1622 as part of the Counter-Reformation, called the Congregatio de Propaganda Fide (Congregation for Propagating the Faith), or informally simply Propaganda.[3][7] Its activity was aimed at 'propagating' the Catholic faith in non-Catholic countries.[3]

From the 1790s, the term began being used also to refer to propaganda in secular activities.[3] The term began taking a pejorative or negative connotation in the mid-19th century, when it was used in the political sphere.[3]

History[edit]

Primitive forms of propaganda have been a human activity as far back as reliable recorded evidence exists. The Behistun Inscription (c. 515 BC) detailing the rise of Darius I to the Persianthrone is viewed by most historians as an early example of propaganda.[8] Another striking example of propaganda during ancient history is the last Roman civil wars (44-30 BC) during which Octavian and Mark Antony blamed each other for obscure and degrading origins, cruelty, cowardice, oratorical and literary incompetence, debaucheries, luxury, drunkenness and other slanders.[9] This defamation took the form of uituperatio (Roman rhetorical genre of the invective) which was decisive for shaping the Roman public opinion at this time.

Propaganda during the Reformation, helped by the spread of the printing press throughout Europe, and in particular within Germany, caused new ideas, thoughts, and doctrine to be made available to the public in ways that had never been seen before the 16th century. During the era of the American Revolution, the American colonies had a flourishing network of newspapers and printers who specialized in the topic on behalf of the Patriots (and to a lesser extent on behalf of the Loyalists).[10]

A propaganda newspaper clipping that refers to the Bataan Death March in 1942

The first large-scale and organised propagation of government propaganda was occasioned by the outbreak of war in 1914. After the defeat of Germany in the First World War, military officials such as Erich Ludendorff suggested that British propaganda had been instrumental in their defeat. Adolf Hitler came to echo this view, believing that it had been a primary cause of the collapse of morale and the revolts in the German home front and Navy in 1918 (see also: Dolchstoßlegende). In Mein Kampf (1925) Hitler expounded his theory of propaganda, which provided a powerful base for his rise to power in 1933. Historian Robert Ensor explains that 'Hitler...puts no limit on what can be done by propaganda; people will believe anything, provided they are told it often enough and emphatically enough, and that contradicters are either silenced or smothered in calumny.'[11] Most propaganda in Nazi Germany was produced by the Ministry of Public Enlightenment and Propaganda under Joseph Goebbels. World War II saw continued use of propaganda as a weapon of war, building on the experience of WWI, by Goebbels and the British Political Warfare Executive, as well as the United States Office of War Information.[12]

Anti-religious Soviet propaganda poster, the Russian text reads 'Away with Religious Holidays!'

In the early 20th century, the invention of motion pictures gave propaganda-creators a powerful tool for advancing political and military interests when it came to reaching a broad segment of the population and creating consent or encouraging rejection of the real or imagined enemy. In the years following the October Revolution of 1917, the Soviet government sponsored the Russian film industry with the purpose of making propaganda films (e.g. the 1925 film The Battleship Potemkin glorifies Communist ideals.) In WWII, Nazi filmmakers produced highly emotional films to create popular support for occupying the Sudetenland and attacking Poland. The 1930s and 1940s, which saw the rise of totalitarian states and the Second World War, are arguably the 'Golden Age of Propaganda'. Leni Riefenstahl, a filmmaker working in Nazi Germany, created one of the best-known propaganda movies, Triumph of the Will. In the US, animation became popular, especially for winning over youthful audiences and aiding the U.S. war effort, e.g.,Der Fuehrer's Face (1942), which ridicules Hitler and advocates the value of freedom. US war films in the early 1940s were designed to create a patriotic mindset and convince viewers that sacrifices needed to be made to defeat the Axis Powers.[13] Polish filmmakers in Great Britain created anti-nazi color film Calling mr. Smith[14][15] (1943) about current nazi crimes in occupied Europe and about lies of nazi propaganda.[16]

The West and the Soviet Union both used propaganda extensively during the Cold War. Both sides used film, television, and radio programming to influence their own citizens, each other, and Third World nations. George Orwell's contemporaneous novels Animal Farm and Nineteen Eighty-Four portray the use of propaganda in fictional dystopian societies. During the Cuban Revolution, Fidel Castro stressed the importance of propaganda.[17][better source needed] Propaganda was used extensively by Communist forces in the Vietnam War as means of controlling people's opinions.[18]

During the Yugoslav wars, propaganda was used as a military strategy by governments of Federal Republic of Yugoslavia and Croatia. Propaganda was used to create fear and hatred, and particularly incite the Serb population against the other ethnicities (Bosniaks, Croats, Albanians and other non-Serbs). Serb media made a great effort in justifying, revising or denying mass war crimes committed by Serb forces during these wars.[19]

Public perceptions[edit]

In the early 20th century the term propaganda was used by the founders of the nascent public relations industry to refer to their people. Literally translated from the Latingerundive as 'things that must be disseminated', in some cultures the term is neutral or even positive, while in others the term has acquired a strong negative connotation. The connotations of the term 'propaganda' can also vary over time. For example, in Portuguese and some Spanish language speaking countries, particularly in the Southern Cone, the word 'propaganda' usually refers to the most common manipulative media – 'advertising'.

Poster of the 19th-century Scandinavist movement

In English, propaganda was originally a neutral term for the dissemination of information in favor of any given cause. During the 20th century, however, the term acquired a thoroughly negative meaning in western countries, representing the intentional dissemination of often false, but certainly 'compelling' claims to support or justify political actions or ideologies. According to Harold Lasswell, the term began to fall out of favor due to growing public suspicion of propaganda in the wake of its use during World War I by the Creel Committee in the United States and the Ministry of Information in Britain: Writing in 1928, Lasswell observed, 'In democratic countries the official propaganda bureau was looked upon with genuine alarm, for fear that it might be suborned to party and personal ends. The outcry in the United States against Mr. Creel's famous Bureau of Public Information (or 'Inflammation') helped to din into the public mind the fact that propaganda existed. ... The public's discovery of propaganda has led to a great of lamentation over it. Propaganda has become an epithet of contempt and hate, and the propagandists have sought protective coloration in such names as 'public relations council,' 'specialist in public education,' 'public relations adviser.' '[20] In 1949, political science professor Dayton David McKean wrote, 'After World War I the word came to be applied to 'what you don’t like of the other fellow’s publicity,' as Edward L. Bernays said....'[21]

The term is essentially contested and some have argued for a neutral definition,[22] arguing that ethics depend on intent and context,[23] while others define it as necessarily unethical and negative.[24] Dr Emma Briant defines it as 'the deliberate manipulation of representations (including text, pictures, video, speech etc.) with the intention of producing any effect in the audience (e.g. action or inaction; reinforcement or transformation of feelings, ideas, attitudes or behaviours) that is desired by the propagandist.'[25]

Types[edit]

Identifying propaganda has always been a problem.[26] The main difficulties have involved differentiating propaganda from other types of persuasion, and avoiding a biased approach. Richard Alan Nelson provides a definition of the term: 'Propaganda is neutrally defined as a systematic form of purposeful persuasion that attempts to influence the emotions, attitudes, opinions, and actions of specified target audiences for ideological, political or commercial purposes through the controlled transmission of one-sided messages (which may or may not be factual) via mass and direct media channels.'[27] The definition focuses on the communicative process involved – or more precisely, on the purpose of the process, and allow 'propaganda' to be considered objectively and then interpreted as positive or negative behavior depending on the perspective of the viewer or listener.

Propaganda poster in North Korean primary school

According to historian Zbyněk Zeman, propaganda is defined as either white, grey or black. White propaganda openly discloses its source and intent. Grey propaganda has an ambiguous or non-disclosed source or intent. Black propaganda purports to be published by the enemy or some organization besides its actual origins[28] (compare with black operation, a type of clandestine operation in which the identity of the sponsoring government is hidden). In scale, these different types of propaganda can also be defined by the potential of true and correct information to compete with the propaganda. For example, opposition to white propaganda is often readily found and may slightly discredit the propaganda source. Opposition to grey propaganda, when revealed (often by an inside source), may create some level of public outcry. Opposition to black propaganda is often unavailable and may be dangerous to reveal, because public cognizance of black propaganda tactics and sources would undermine or backfire the very campaign the black propagandist supported.

Propaganda poster in North Korea

The propagandist seeks to change the way people understand an issue or situation for the purpose of changing their actions and expectations in ways that are desirable to the interest group. Propaganda, in this sense, serves as a corollary to censorship in which the same purpose is achieved, not by filling people's minds with approved information, but by preventing people from being confronted with opposing points of view. What sets propaganda apart from other forms of advocacy is the willingness of the propagandist to change people's understanding through deception and confusion rather than persuasion and understanding. The leaders of an organization know the information to be one sided or untrue, but this may not be true for the rank and file members who help to disseminate the propaganda.

From a series of woodcuts (1545) usually referred to as the Papstspotbilder or Papstspottbilder in German or Depictions of the Papacy in English,[29] by Lucas Cranach, commissioned by Martin Luther.[30] Title: Kissing the Pope's Feet.[31] German peasants respond to a papal bull of Pope Paul III. Caption reads: 'Don't frighten us Pope, with your ban, and don't be such a furious man. Otherwise we shall turn around and show you our rears.'[32][33]

Religious[edit]

Propaganda was often used to influence opinions and beliefs on religious issues, particularly during the split between the Roman Catholic Church and the Protestant churches.

More in line with the religious roots of the term, propaganda is also used widely in the debates about new religious movements (NRMs), both by people who defend them and by people who oppose them. The latter pejoratively call these NRMs cults. Anti-cult activists and Christian countercult activists accuse the leaders of what they consider cults of using propaganda extensively to recruit followers and keep them. Some social scientists, such as the late Jeffrey Hadden, and CESNUR affiliated scholars accuse ex-members of 'cults' and the anti-cult movement of making these unusual religious movements look bad without sufficient reasons.[34][35]

Wartime[edit]

A US Office for War Information poster uses stereotyped imagery to encourage Americans to work hard to contribute to the war effort

Post–World War II usage of the word 'propaganda' more typically refers to political or nationalist uses of these techniques or to the promotion of a set of ideas.

Propaganda is a powerful weapon in war; it is used to dehumanize and create hatred toward a supposed enemy, either internal or external, by creating a false image in the mind of soldiers and citizens. This can be done by using derogatory or racist terms (e.g., the racist terms 'Jap' and 'gook' used during World War II and the Vietnam War, respectively), avoiding some words or language or by making allegations of enemy atrocities. Most propaganda efforts in wartime require the home population to feel the enemy has inflicted an injustice, which may be fictitious or may be based on facts (e.g., the sinking of the passenger ship RMS Lusitania by the German Navy in World War I). The home population must also believe that the cause of their nation in the war is just. In NATO doctrine, propaganda is defined as 'Any information, ideas, doctrines, or special appeals disseminated to influence the opinion, emotions, attitudes, or behaviour of any specified group in order to benefit the sponsor either directly or indirectly.'[36] Within this perspective, information provided does not need to be necessarily false, but must be instead relevant to specific goals of the 'actor' or 'system' that performs it.

Propaganda is also one of the methods used in psychological warfare, which may also involve false flag operations in which the identity of the operatives is depicted as those of an enemy nation (e.g., The Bay of Pigs invasion used CIA planes painted in Cuban Air Force markings). The term propaganda may also refer to false information meant to reinforce the mindsets of people who already believe as the propagandist wishes (e.g., During the First World War, the main purpose of British propaganda was to encourage men join the army, and women to work in the country's industry. The propaganda posters were used, because radios and TVs were not very common at that time.).[37] The assumption is that, if people believe something false, they will constantly be assailed by doubts. Since these doubts are unpleasant (see cognitive dissonance), people will be eager to have them extinguished, and are therefore receptive to the reassurances of those in power. For this reason propaganda is often addressed to people who are already sympathetic to the agenda or views being presented. This process of reinforcement uses an individual's predisposition to self-select 'agreeable' information sources as a mechanism for maintaining control over populations.

Britannia arm-in-arm with Uncle Sam symbolizes the British-American alliance in World War I.

Propaganda may be administered in insidious ways. For instance, disparaging disinformation about the history of certain groups or foreign countries may be encouraged or tolerated in the educational system. Since few people actually double-check what they learn at school, such disinformation will be repeated by journalists as well as parents, thus reinforcing the idea that the disinformation item is really a 'well-known fact', even though no one repeating the myth is able to point to an authoritative source. The disinformation is then recycled in the media and in the educational system, without the need for direct governmental intervention on the media. Such permeating propaganda may be used for political goals: by giving citizens a false impression of the quality or policies of their country, they may be incited to reject certain proposals or certain remarks or ignore the experience of others.

In the Soviet Union during the Second World War, the propaganda designed to encourage civilians was controlled by Stalin, who insisted on a heavy-handed style that educated audiences easily saw was inauthentic. On the other hand, the unofficial rumours about German atrocities were well founded and convincing.[38] Stalin was a Georgian who spoke Russian with a heavy accent. That would not do for a national hero so starting in the 1930s all new visual portraits of Stalin were retouched to erase his Georgian facial characteristics and make him a more generalized Soviet hero. Only his eyes and famous mustache remained unaltered. Zhores Medvedev and Roy Medvedev say his 'majestic new image was devised appropriately to depict the leader of all times and of all peoples.'[39]

Article 20 of the International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights prohibits any propaganda for war as well as any advocacy of national or religious hatred that constitutes incitement to discrimination, hostility or violence by law.[40]

Naturally, the common people don't want war; neither in Russia nor in England nor in America, nor for that matter in Germany. That is understood. But, after all, it is the leaders of the country who determine the policy and it is always a simple matter to drag the people along, whether it is a democracy or a fascist dictatorship or a Parliament or a Communist dictatorship. The people can always be brought to the bidding of the leaders. That is easy. All you have to do is tell them they are being attacked and denounce the pacifists for lack of patriotism and exposing the country to danger. It works the same way in any country.

— Hermann Göring[41]

Advertising[edit]

Propaganda shares techniques with advertising and public relations, each of which can be thought of as propaganda that promotes a commercial product or shapes the perception of an organization, person, or brand.

Simply Simon Thoroughbred Gelding

World War I propaganda poster for enlistment in the U.S. Army

Journalistic theory generally holds that news items should be objective, giving the reader an accurate background and analysis of the subject at hand. On the other hand, advertisements evolved from the traditional commercial advertisements to include also a new type in the form of paid articles or broadcasts disguised as news. These generally present an issue in a very subjective and often misleading light, primarily meant to persuade rather than inform. Normally they use only subtle propaganda techniques and not the more obvious ones used in traditional commercial advertisements. If the reader believes that a paid advertisement is in fact a news item, the message the advertiser is trying to communicate will be more easily 'believed' or 'internalized'. Such advertisements are considered obvious examples of 'covert' propaganda because they take on the appearance of objective information rather than the appearance of propaganda, which is misleading. Federal law specifically mandates that any advertisement appearing in the format of a news item must state that the item is in fact a paid advertisement.

Politics[edit]

Propaganda has become more common in political contexts, in particular to refer to certain efforts sponsored by governments, political groups, but also often covert interests. In the early 20th century, propaganda was exemplified in the form of party slogans. Propaganda also has much in common with public information campaigns by governments, which are intended to encourage or discourage certain forms of behavior (such as wearing seat belts, not smoking, not littering and so forth). Again, the emphasis is more political in propaganda. Propaganda can take the form of leaflets, posters, TV and radio broadcasts and can also extend to any other medium. In the case of the United States, there is also an important legal (imposed by law) distinction between advertising (a type of overt propaganda) and what the Government Accountability Office (GAO), an arm of the United States Congress, refers to as 'covert propaganda'.

Roderick Hindery argues[42] that propaganda exists on the political left, and right, and in mainstream centrist parties. Hindery further argues that debates about most social issues can be productively revisited in the context of asking 'what is or is not propaganda?' Not to be overlooked is the link between propaganda, indoctrination, and terrorism/counterterrorism. He argues that threats to destroy are often as socially disruptive as physical devastation itself.

Anti-communist propaganda in a 1947 comic book published by the Catechetical Guild Educational Society warning of 'the dangers of a Communist takeover'

Since 9/11 and the appearance of greater media fluidity, propaganda institutions, practices and legal frameworks have been evolving in the US and Britain. Dr Emma Louise Briant shows how this included expansion and integration of the apparatus cross-government and details attempts to coordinate the forms of propaganda for foreign and domestic audiences, with new efforts in strategic communication.[43] These were subject to contestation within the US Government, resisted by PentagonPublic Affairs and critiqued by some scholars.[44] The National Defense Authorization Act for Fiscal Year 2013 (section 1078 (a)) amended the US Information and Educational Exchange Act of 1948 (popularly referred to as the Smith-Mundt Act) and the Foreign Relations Authorization Act of 1987, allowing for materials produced by the State Department and the Broadcasting Board of Governors (BBG) to be released within U.S. borders for the Archivist of the United States. The Smith-Mundt Act, as amended, provided that 'the Secretary and the Broadcasting Board of Governors shall make available to the Archivist of the United States, for domestic distribution, motion pictures, films, videotapes, and other material 12 years after the initial dissemination of the material abroad (...) Nothing in this section shall be construed to prohibit the Department of State or the Broadcasting Board of Governors from engaging in any medium or form of communication, either directly or indirectly, because a United States domestic audience is or may be thereby exposed to program material, or based on a presumption of such exposure.' Public concerns were raised upon passage due to the relaxation of prohibitions of domestic propaganda in the United States.[45]

Techniques[edit]

Anti-capitalist propaganda

Common media for transmitting propaganda messages include news reports, government reports, historical revision, junk science, books, leaflets, movies, radio, television, and posters. Some propaganda campaigns follow a strategic transmission pattern to indoctrinate the target group. This may begin with a simple transmission, such as a leaflet or advertisement dropped from a plane or an advertisement. Generally these messages will contain directions on how to obtain more information, via a web site, hot line, radio program, etc. (as it is seen also for selling purposes among other goals). The strategy intends to initiate the individual from information recipient to information seeker through reinforcement, and then from information seeker to opinion leader through indoctrination.[46]

A number of techniques based in social psychological research are used to generate propaganda. Many of these same techniques can be found under logical fallacies, since propagandists use arguments that, while sometimes convincing, are not necessarily valid.

Some time has been spent analyzing the means by which the propaganda messages are transmitted. That work is important but it is clear that information dissemination strategies become propaganda strategies only when coupled with propagandistic messages. Identifying these messages is a necessary prerequisite to study the methods by which those messages are spread.

Propaganda can also be turned on its makers. For example postage stamps have frequently been tools for government advertising, such as North Korea's extensive issues.[47] The presence of Stalin on numerous Soviet stamps is another example.[48] During the Third ReichHitler frequently appeared on postage stamps in Germany and some of the occupied nations. A British program to parody these, and other Nazi-inspired stamps, involved air dropping them into Germany on letters containing anti-Nazi literature.[49][50]

Models[edit]

Social psychology[edit]

Public reading of the anti-Semitic newspaper Der Stürmer, Worms, Germany, 1935

The field of social psychology includes the study of persuasion. Social psychologists can be sociologists or psychologists. The field includes many theories and approaches to understanding persuasion. For example, communication theory points out that people can be persuaded by the communicator's credibility, expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. The elaboration likelihood model as well as heuristic models of persuasion suggest that a number of factors (e.g., the degree of interest of the recipient of the communication), influence the degree to which people allow superficial factors to persuade them. Nobel Prize–winning psychologist Herbert A. Simon won the Nobel prize for his theory that people are cognitive misers. That is, in a society of mass information, people are forced to make decisions quickly and often superficially, as opposed to logically.

According to William W. Biddle's 1931 article 'A psychological definition of propaganda', '[t]he four principles followed in propaganda are: (1) rely on emotions, never argue; (2) cast propaganda into the pattern of 'we' versus an 'enemy'; (3) reach groups as well as individuals; (4) hide the propagandist as much as possible.'[51]

Herman and Chomsky[edit]

Early 20th-century depiction of a 'European Anarchist' attempting to destroy the Statue of Liberty

The propaganda model is a theory advanced by Edward S. Herman and Noam Chomsky which argues systemic biases in the mass media and seeks to explain them in terms of structural economic causes:

The 20th century has been characterized by three developments of great political importance: the growth of democracy, the growth of corporate power, and the growth of corporate propaganda as a means of protecting corporate power against democracy.[52][53]

First presented in their 1988 book Manufacturing Consent: The Political Economy of the Mass Media, the propaganda model views the private media as businesses selling a product – readers and audiences (rather than news) – to other businesses (advertisers) and relying primarily on government and corporate information and propaganda. The theory postulates five general classes of 'filters' that determine the type of news that is presented in news media: Ownership of the medium, the medium's Funding, Sourcing of the news, Flak, and anti-communist ideology.

The first three (ownership, funding, and sourcing) are generally regarded by the authors as being the most important. Although the model was based mainly on the characterization of United States media, Chomsky and Herman believe the theory is equally applicable to any country that shares the basic economic structure and organizing principles the model postulates as the cause of media bias.

Simon

Children[edit]

A 1938 propaganda of the New State depicting Brazilian President Getúlio Vargas flanked by children. The text on the bottom right of this poster translates as: 'Children! Learning, at home and in school, the cult of the Fatherland, you will bring all chances of success to life. Only love builds and, strongly loving Brazil, you will lead it to the greatest of destinies among Nations, fulfilling the desires of exaltation nestled in every Brazilian heart.'
Poster promoting the Nicaraguan Sandinistas. The text reads: 'Sandinista children: Toño, Delia and Rodolfo are in the Association of Sandinista Children. Sandinista children use a neckerchief. They participate in the revolution and are very studious.'

Of all the potential targets for propaganda, children are the most vulnerable because they are the least prepared with the critical reasoning and contextual comprehension they need to determine whether a message is propaganda or not. The attention children give their environment during development, due to the process of developing their understanding of the world, causes them to absorb propaganda indiscriminately. Also, children are highly imitative: studies by Albert Bandura, Dorothea Ross and Sheila A. Ross in the 1960s indicated that, to a degree, socialization, formal education and standardized television programming can be seen as using propaganda for the purpose of indoctrination. The use of propaganda in schools was highly prevalent during the 1930s and 1940s in Germany in the form of the Hitler Youth.

Simply Simon Pizza

John Taylor Gatto asserts that modern schooling in the USA is designed to 'dumb us down' in order to turn children into material suitable to work in factories. This ties into the Herman & Chomsky thesis of rise of Corporate Power, and its use in creating educational systems which serve its purposes against those of democracy.

Anti-Semitic propaganda for children[edit]

In Nazi Germany, the education system was thoroughly co-opted to indoctrinate the German youth with anti-Semitic ideology. This was accomplished through the National Socialist Teachers League, of which 97% of all German teachers were members in 1937. The League encouraged the teaching of racial theory. Picture books for children such as Don't Trust A Fox in A Green Meadow or The Word of A Jew, Der Giftpilz (translated into English as The Poisonous Mushroom) and The Poodle-Pug-Dachshund-Pincher were widely circulated (over 100,000 copies of Don't Trust A Fox... were circulated during the late 1930s) and contained depictions of Jews as devils, child molesters and other morally charged figures. Slogans such as 'Judas the Jew betrayed Jesus the German to the Jews' were recited in class.[54] The following is an example of a propagandistic math problem recommended by the National Socialist Essence of Education: 'The Jews are aliens in Germany—in 1933 there were 66,606,000 inhabitants in the German Reich, of whom 499,682 (.75%) were Jews.'[55]

See also[edit]

  • Quis custodiet ipsos custodes? ('Who will guard the guards themselves?')

References[edit]

  1. ^'Pravda za Uroša Predića!'. e-novine.com. Retrieved 5 May 2015.
  2. ^ abSmith, Bruce L. (17 February 2016). 'Propaganda'. Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. Retrieved 23 April 2016.
  3. ^ abcdeDiggs-Brown, Barbara (2011) Strategic Public Relations: Audience Focused Practice p. 48
  4. ^Martin, Everett Dean, Are We Victims of Propaganda, Our Invisible Masters: A Debate with Edward Bernays, The Forum, pp. 142–150, March 1929 (1929)
  5. ^'Martin Bernays debate'(PDF). postflaviana.org.
  6. ^Oxford dictionary.
  7. ^'Online Etymology Dictionary'. Retrieved 6 March 2015.
  8. ^Nagle, D. Brendan; Stanley M Burstein (2009). The Ancient World: Readings in Social and Cultural History. Pearson Education. p. 133. ISBN978-0-205-69187-6.
  9. ^Borgies, Loïc (2016). Le conflit propagandiste entre Octavien et Marc Antoine. De l'usage politique de la uituperatio entre 44 et 30 a. C. n. ISBN978-90-429-3459-7.
  10. ^Cole, Richard G, 1975, 'The Reformation in Print: German Pamphlets and Propaganda. Archiv für Reformationsgeschichte – Archive for Reformation History', Pg. 93–102
  11. ^Robert Ensor in David Thomson, ed., The New Cambridge Modern History: volume XII The Era of Violence 1890–1945 (1st edition 1960), p 84.
  12. ^Fox, J. C., 2007, 'Film propaganda in Britain and Nazi Germany : World War II cinema.', Oxford:Berg.
  13. ^Philip M. Taylor, 1990, 'Munitions of the mind: A history of propaganda”, Pg. 170.
  14. ^'Calling Mr. Smith – LUX'.
  15. ^'Calling Mr Smith – Centre Pompidou'.
  16. ^'Franciszka and Stefan Themerson: Calling Mr. Smith (1943) – artincinema'. 21 June 2015.
  17. ^prudentiapolitica. 'Prudentia Politica'. Retrieved 6 March 2015.
  18. ^[1] Vietnamese propaganda reflections from 1945 to 2000
  19. ^'Serbian Propaganda: A Closer Look'. 12 April 1999. NOAH ADAMS: The European Center for War, Peace and the News Media, based in London, has received word from Belgrade that no pictures of mass Albanian refugees have been shown at all, and that the Kosovo humanitarian catastrophe is only referred to as the one made up or over-emphasised by Western propaganda.
  20. ^pp. 260–261, 'The Function of the Propagandist', International Journal of Ethics, 38 (no. 3): pp. 258–268.
  21. ^p. 113, Party and Pressure Politics, Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1949.
  22. ^Briant, Emma, L (2015) Propaganda and Counter-terrorism: Strategies for Global Change, Manchester: Manchester University Press p 9 & Taylor, Phil M. (2002), ‘Debate: Strategic Communications or Democratic Propaganda?’, in Journalism Studies, Vol. 3, No. 3, pp. 437–452.
  23. ^Briant, Emma, L (2015) Propaganda and Counter-terrorism: Strategies for Global Change, Manchester: Manchester University Press
  24. ^Doob, L.W. (1949), Public Opinion and Propaganda, London: Cresset Press p 240
  25. ^Briant, Emma, L (2015) Propaganda and Counter-terrorism: Strategies for Global Change, Manchester: Manchester University Press p 9
  26. ^Daniel J Schwindt, The Case Against the Modern World: A Crash Course in Traditionalist Thought, 2016, pp. 202–204.
  27. ^Richard Alan Nelson, A Chronology and Glossary of Propaganda in the United States (1996) pp. 232–233
  28. ^Zeman, Zbynek (1978). Selling the War. Orbis Publishing. ISBN978-0-85613-312-1.
  29. ^Oberman, Heiko Augustinus (1 January 1994). The Impact of the Reformation: Essays. Wm. B. Eerdmans Publishing. ISBN9780802807328 – via Google Books.
  30. ^Luther's Last Battles: Politics And Polemics 1531-46 By Mark U. Edwards, Jr. Fortress Press, 2004. ISBN978-0-8006-3735-4
  31. ^In Latin, the title reads 'Hic oscula pedibus papae figuntur'
  32. ^'Nicht Bapst: nicht schreck uns mit deim ban, Und sey nicht so zorniger man. Wir thun sonst ein gegen wehre, Und zeigen dirs Bel vedere'
  33. ^Mark U. Edwards, Jr., Luther's Last Battles: Politics And Polemics 1531-46 (2004), p. 199
  34. ^'The Religious Movements Page: Conceptualizing 'Cult' and 'Sect''. Archived from the original on 7 February 2006. Retrieved 4 December 2005.
  35. ^'Polish Anti-Cult Movement (Koscianska) – CESNUR'. Retrieved 4 December 2005.
  36. ^North Atlantic Treaty Organization, Nato Standardization Agency Aap-6 – Glossary of terms and definitions, p 188.
  37. ^Callanan, James D. The Evolution of The CIA's Covert Action Mission, 1947–1963. Durham University. 1999.
  38. ^Karel C. Berkhoff, Motherland in Danger: Soviet Propaganda during World War II (2012) excerpt and text search
  39. ^Zhores A. Medvedev and (2003). The Unknown Stalin. p. 248. ISBN9781860647680.
  40. ^'International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights'. United Nations Human Rights: Office of the High Commissioner for Human Rights. United Nations. Retrieved 2 September 2015.
  41. ^Gustave Gilbert's Nuremberg Diary(1947). In an interview with Gilbert in Göring's jail cell during the Nuremberg War Crimes Trials (18 April 1946)
  42. ^Hindery, Roderick R., Indoctrination and Self-deception or Free and Critical Thought? (2001)
  43. ^Briant (April 2015). 'Allies and Audiences Evolving Strategies in Defense and Intelligence Propaganda'. The International Journal of Press/Politics. 20 (2): 145–165. doi:10.1177/1940161214552031.
  44. ^Briant, Emma (2015). Propaganda and Counter-terrorism: strategies for global change. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
  45. ^'Smith-Mundt Act'. 'Anti-Propaganda' Ban Repealed, Freeing State Dept. To Direct Its Broadcasting Arm at American Citizens. Techdirt. Retrieved 1 June 2016.
  46. ^Garth S. Jowett and Victoria J.: O'Donnell, Propaganda & Persuasion (5th ed. 2011)
  47. ^North Korea’s America-hating postage stamps are mini masterpieces of anti-imperialist propaganda
  48. ^Stalin on Stamps and other Philatelic Materials:Design, Propaganda, Politics
  49. ^Propaganda and Espionage Philately
  50. ^10 WWII Stamp Forgeries Used as Psychological Warfare
  51. ^Biddle, William W. A psychological definition of propaganda. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol 26(3), Oct 1931, 283–295.
  52. ^'Letter from Noam Chomsky' to Covert Action Quarterly, quoting Alex Carey, Australian social scientist, 'Letter from Noam Chomsky'. Archived from the original on 10 July 2012. Retrieved 1 April 2007.
  53. ^'Review of Alex Carey, Taking the Risk out of Democracy: Propaganda in the US and Australia'. Retrieved 6 March 2015.
  54. ^Mills, Mary. 'Propaganda and Children During the Hitler Years'. Jewish Virtual Library. https://www.jewishvirtuallibrary.org/jsource/Holocaust/propchil.html
  55. ^Hirsch, Herbert. Genocide and the Politics of Memory. Chapel Hill & London: University of North Carolina Press, 1995. p. 119.

Sources[edit]

  • 'Appendix I: PSYOP Techniques'. Psychological Operations Field Manual No. 33-1. Washington, D.C.: Department of the Army. 31 August 1979. Archived from the original on 24 May 2001.CS1 maint: BOT: original-url status unknown (link)
  • Bytwerk, Randall L. (2004). Bending Spines: The Propagandas of Nazi Germany and the German Democratic Republic. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press. ISBN978-0-87013-710-5.
  • Edwards, John Carver (1991). Berlin Calling: American Broadcasters in Service to the Third Reich. New York: Praeger. ISBN978-0-275-93905-2.
  • Hindery, Roderick. 'The Anatomy of Propaganda within Religious Terrorism'. Humanist (March–April 2003): 16–19.
  • Howe, Ellic (1982). The Black Game: British Subversive Operations Against the German During the Second World War. London: Futura.
  • Huxley, Aldous (1958). Brave New World Revisited. New York: Harper. ISBN978-0-06-080984-3.
  • Jowett, Garth S.; O'Donnell, Victoria (2006). Propaganda and Persuasion (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, Inc. ISBN978-1-4129-0897-9.
  • Le Bon, Gustave (1895). The Crowd: A Study of the Popular Mind. ISBN978-0-14-004531-4.
  • Linebarger, Paul M. A. (1948). Psychological Warfare. Washington, D.C.: Infantry Journal Press. ISBN978-0-405-04755-8.
  • Nelson, Richard Alan (1996). A Chronology and Glossary of Propaganda in the United States. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. ISBN978-0-313-29261-3.
  • Shirer, William L. (1942). Berlin Diary: The Journal of a Foreign Correspondent, 1934–1941. New York: Albert A. Knopf. ISBN978-5-9524-0081-8.
  • Young, Emma (10 October 2001). 'Psychological warfare waged in Afghanistan'. New Scientist. Archived from the original on 13 February 2002. Retrieved 5 August 2010.

Further reading[edit]

Simply Simon Malls

Books[edit]

  • Altheide, David L. & John M. Johnson. Bureaucratic Propaganda. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1980.
  • Bernays, Edward. Propaganda. New York: H. Liveright, 1928. (See also version of text at website www.historyisaweapon.com: 'Propaganda.')
  • Borgies, Loïc. Le conflit propagandiste entre Octavien et Marc Antoine: De l'usage politique de la uituperatio entre 44 et 30 a. C. n.. Brussels: Latomus, 2016.
  • Brown, J.A.C. Techniques of Persuasion: From Propaganda to Brainwashing. Harmondsworth: Pelican, 1963.
  • Chomsky, Noam & Herman Edward. Manufacturing Consent: The Political Economy of the Mass Media. New York: Pantheon Books. (1988)
  • Chomsky, Noam. Media Control: The Spectacular Achievements of Propaganda. Seven Stories Press, 1997.
  • Cole, Robert. Propaganda in Twentieth Century War and Politics: An Annotated Bibliography. London: Scarecrow, 1996.
  • Cole, Robert, ed. Encyclopedia of Propaganda. 3 vols. Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe, 1998.
  • Combs James E. & Nimmo Dan, The New Propaganda: The Dictatorship of Palaver in Contemporary Politics. White Plains, N.Y. Longman. (1993)
  • Cull, Nicholas John, Culbert, and Welch, eds. Propaganda and Mass Persuasion: A Historical Encyclopedia, 1500 to the Present (2003)
  • Cunningham Stanley B. The Idea of Propaganda: A Reconstruction. Westport, Conn.: Praeger, 2002.
  • Cunningham Stanley B. 'Reflections on the Interface Between Propaganda and Religion', in The Future of Religion, eds. P. Rennick, S. Cunningham, & R.H. Johnson. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Pub., 2010, pp. 83–96.
  • Dimitri Kitsikis, Propagande et pressions en politique internationale, Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1963, 537 pages.
  • Ellul, Jacques, Propaganda: The Formation of Men's Attitudes. (1965).
  • Hale, Oron James. Publicity and Diplomacy: With Special Reference to England and Germany, 1890–1914 (1940) online
  • Jowett, Garth S. & Victoria O'Donnell. Propaganda and Persuasion, 6th edn. California: Sage Publications, 2014. A detailed overview of the history, function, and analyses of propaganda.
  • Lohrey, Andrew, ed. Taking the Risk out of Democracy: Corporate Propaganda versus Freedom and Liberty. Urbana, Ill.: University of Illinois Press, 1997.
  • Marlin, Randal. Propaganda & The Ethics of Persuasion. Orchard Park, New York: Broadview Press, 2002.
  • McCombs, M. E. & D. L. Shaw. “The agenda-setting function of mass media”, Public Opinion Quarterly 36, no. 2 (1972): 176–187.
  • Moran, T. 'Propaganda as Pseudocommunication', Et Cetera 2 (1979): 181–197.
  • Nelson, Richard Allen. A Chronology and Glossary of Propaganda in the United States. Westport, Conn.: Greenwood Press, 1996.
  • Pratkanis, Anthony & Elliot Aronson. Age of Propaganda: The Everyday Use and Abuse of Persuasion. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company, 1992.
  • Rutherford, Paul, Endless Propaganda: The Advertising of Public Goods. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (2000)
  • Rutherford, Paul, Weapons of Mass Persuasion: Marketing the War Against Iraq. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, 2004.
  • Shanahan, James, ed. Propaganda without Propagandists? Six Case Studies in U.S. Propaganda. Hampton Press, 2001.
  • Shaw Jeffrey M., Illusions of Freedom: Thomas Merton and Jacques Ellul on Technology and the Human Condition. Eugene, OR: Wipf and Stock. ISBN978-1625640581 (2014)
  • Snow, Nancy (10 March 2014). Propaganda and American Democracy. Baton Rouge: LSU Press. ISBN978-0-8071-5415-1.
  • Snow, Nancy (4 January 2011). Propaganda, Inc.: Selling America's Culture to the World. New York: Seven Stories Press. ISBN978-1-60980-082-6.
  • Sproule J. Michael, Channels of Propaganda. Bloomington, IN: EDINFO Press. (1994)
  • Stanley, Jason (2016). How Propaganda Works. Princeton University Press. ISBN978-0691173429.
  • Stauber, John & Sheldon Rampton. Toxic Sludge Is Good for You! Lies, Damn Lies and the Public Relations Industry. Monroe, Maine: Common Courage Press, 1995.

Essays and articles[edit]

  • Brown, John H.. 'Two Ways of Looking at Propaganda' (2006)
  • Kosar, Kevin R., Public Relations and Propaganda: Restrictions on Executive Branch Activities, CRS Report RL32750, February 2005.
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Propaganda.
Simon
Wikiquote has quotations related to: Propaganda
Retrieved from 'https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Propaganda&oldid=912346652'

Simon Aronson Simply Simon Pdf Viewer Online

Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645 – 662
Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat Catherine Gooda,*, Joshua Aronsonb,1, Michael Inzlichtb a
Department of Psychology, Columbia University, 405 Schermerhorn Hall, 1190 Amsterdam Avenue, New York, NY 10027, USA b New York University, East Building, 239 Greene Street, 537F, New York, NY 10003, USA
Abstract Standardized tests continue to generate gender and race gaps in achievement despite decades of national attention. Research on ‘‘stereotype threat’’ (Steele & Aronson, 1995) suggests that these gaps may be partly due to stereotypes that impugn the math abilities of females and the intellectual abilities of Black, Hispanic, and low-income students. A field experiment was performed to test methods of helping female, minority, and low-income adolescents overcome the anxiety-inducing effects of stereotype threat and, consequently, improve their standardized test scores. Specifically, seventh-grade students in the experimental conditions were mentored by college students who encouraged them either to view intelligence as malleable or to attribute academic difficulties in the seventh grade to the novelty of the educational setting. Results showed that females in both experimental conditions earned significantly higher math standardized test scores than females in the control condition. Similarly, the students—who were largely minority and low-income adolescents—in the experimental conditions earned significantly higher reading standardized test scores than students in the control condition. D 2003 Published by Elsevier Inc. Keywords: Stereotype threat; Adolescents; Standardized tests; Mathematics; Reading; Gender differences; Lowincome students; Minority students; Attributions; Beliefs about intelligence
* Corresponding author. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (C. Good), [email protected] (J. Aronson), [email protected] (M. Inzlicht). 1 Tel.: +1-212-998-5543; fax: +1-212-995-4563. 0193-3973/$ – see front matter D 2003 Published by Elsevier Inc. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.002
646
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
1. Introduction When Neil de Grasse Tyson received his doctoral degree from Columbia University in 1991 he became the seventh African American astrophysicist out of 4000 astrophysicists nationwide. In his convocation address delivered at Columbia’s graduation ceremonies, he summarized his life path by noting: In the perception of society my athletic talents are genetic; I am a likely mugger/rapist; my academic failures are expected; and my academic successes are attributed to others. To spend most of my life fighting these attitudes levies an emotional tax that is a form of intellectual emasculation (de Grasse Tyson, 1991). The ‘‘emotional tax’’ de Grasse Tyson referred to aptly describes the undermining role that stereotypes can play in the intellectual lives of African American students—and, indeed, of anyone who contends with stereotypes that question his or her abilities. The psychological literature has taught us much about how negative stereotypes can contribute to intellectual underperformance among stigmatized students (e.g., Steele, 1997; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002). It has offered much less, however, about what can be done to lift the emotional tax levied upon stigmatized students. In this article, we hope to address this shortcoming by introducing an intervention program designed to remedy stereotype-based underperformance at a critical period in a student’s burgeoning academic life—the transition to junior high school. 1.1. The academic performance of minorities and females The academic underperformance of Black and Hispanic students and the underrepresentation of girls and women in the mathematics and science domains have long troubled people concerned with educational inequities. Each year, statistics from statewide and national tests reaffirm the disturbing pattern of underachievement. For example, compared to White and Asian students, Black students receive lower grades and have higher dropout rates at practically every level of schooling. Yet despite decades of national concern, recent data suggest that the disparities are not likely to disappear soon. Indeed, the gap in high school GPA between Black and White students actually increased in 2002 (The College Board, 2002), and Black students still obtain lower scores on standardized tests of reading, math, and science (Jencks & Phillips, 1998). Because standardized test scores are the preferred standard for college admissions, it is not surprising that Black students make up less than 10% of admissions to 4-year colleges (National Center for Education Statistics, 2003). Although Hispanic students fare somewhat better, their school achievement and standardized test scores also tend to lag substantially behind that of White and Asian students (see Romo & Falbo, 1995). A similar pattern exists for girls and young women in the areas of math and science. For example, females lag behind boys on the math section of the SAT by as much as 35 points. This performance gap has decreased by a mere 3 points in the past 10 years (The College Board, 2002) despite the numerous programs designed to increase females’ math and science outcomes, such as Expanding Your Horizons (http://www.expandingyourhorizons.org/).
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
647
Much of the psychological and educational research examining the various factors presumed to underlie these race and gender gaps have concluded that sociological factors, such as teachers’ expectations, are often to blame (e.g., Eccles & Jacobs, 1992; Jencks & Phillips, 1998; Romo & Falbo, 1995; Sadker & Sadker, 1994; Valencia, 1997). Recent research in social psychology, however, has demonstrated that these gaps may be a product of a more general cognitive process that may be, as a result, more amenable to intervention than previously thought (Aronson et al., 1999; Aronson, Quinn, & Spencer, 1998; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995; Steele et al., 2002). Specifically, this research suggests that individuals may suffer negative performance outcomes (lower standardized test scores and less engagement with academics) because they are burdened by the prospect of confirming cultural stereotypes impugning their intellectual and academic abilities. Calling this burden ‘‘stereotype threat,’’ Steele and Aronson (1995) conducted research demonstrating that this phenomenon can be felt as a physiological arousal (BenZeev, Fein, & Inzlicht, 2003; Blascovich, Spencer, Quinn, & Steele, 2001) that often results in substantial decrements in intellectual performance. For example, Steele and Aronson showed that inducing stereotype threat—by presenting a test as a measure of intellectual ability, or by asking test takers to indicate their race before the test—can significantly undermine African Americans’ performance on intellectual tasks. They also showed that reducing stereotype threat—by convincing test takers that the test is not being used to measure their abilities—can significantly improve African Americans’ performance, dramatically narrowing the race gap. Building on these findings, subsequent experimental work has established that stereotype threat can undermine the academic performance of females in math (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Spencer et al., 1999; Good, Aronson, & Harder, 1999), students from low socioeconomic backgrounds (Croizet & Claire, 1998) and, in fact, any group that contends with negative stereotypes about their intellectual abilities (Aronson et al., 1999). This research suggests that underperformance results, in part, from pejorative interpretations of failure, which are facilitated by the stereotype (Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002). These pejorative interpretations, which suggest low ability rather than surmountable challenges, add stress and self-doubt to students’ educational experiences and diminish their sense of belonging to the academic arena (Good & Dweck, 2003). Two factors that contribute to stereotype threat can help us determine the most effective strategies for helping academically stigmatized individuals overcome vulnerability to stereotype-based underperformance. First, research indicates that evaluative scrutiny is at the heart of most situations that evoke stereotype threat. Being evaluated in a stereotyped domain is sufficient to trigger the trademark responses associated with stereotype threat—lack of enjoyment of the educational process, increased anxiety and stress, and, ultimately, underperformance. Second, group composition—the racial or gender mix in a room of test takers— also can trigger stereotype-relevant thoughts, and thus vulnerability to stereotype threat (Inzlicht & Ben Zeev, 2000, in press) because group composition can make salient one’s social identity and the stereotypes associated with that identity. Clearly, these two conditions—evaluative scrutiny and identity salience—are characteristic of most testing environments in which students find themselves, such as Advanced Placement examinations, the SAT, the GRE, and the like. Group salience is endemic to these testing
648
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
situations, not only because students often indicate their race and gender before taking the test, but also because minority and female students take the tests in the presence of White students and males. And, undoubtedly, students fully recognize that their performance on these tests can have important implications for their academic futures, determining college credit, scholarships, and school admissions. Consequently, arming students with the means to overcome the stereotype threat they are likely to experience during these tests could potentially reduce the race and gender gaps that have troubled standardized testing for decades. In designing the intervention program for this study, we were driven by two goals. The first was to develop a program for adolescents, for whom educational difficulties can develop quickly and can set the stage for future academic problems. The second goal was to explore the potential of social psychological interventions for improving standardized test performance. Such interventions have successfully boosted test performance in the laboratory (Aronson et al., 1998) and have improved GPAs (Aronson et al., 2002; Wilson & Linville, 1985). However, this past research involved college undergraduates and did not involve actual high-stakes standardized testing. 1.2. Stereotype threat and the transition to junior high school Our desire to implement a program for adolescents led us to focus on the transition to junior high school. Timing our intervention to coincide with adolescents’ transition to junior high school is important for three reasons. First, the transition to junior high school is the time at which most students falter academically, some continuing to struggle throughout their academic life (Eccles, Lord, & Midgley, 1991). Sadly, the decline is particularly steep and there is less likely to be a rebound for girls in math and for minority students more generally. For example, although most students experience some initial difficulties transitioning to a new school environment, several studies report significantly more problems, i.e., suspensions, low academic performance, conflicts with parents, etc., among Black and Latino students than among White students (e.g., Felner, Primavera, & Cauce, 1981; Simmons, Black, & Zhou, 1991). Furthermore, it is during junior high school that early differences in confidence manifest as differences in math performance between males and females. One possible reason for this is the ‘‘stereotype climate’’ that is engendered and reinforced by the middle school setting (Aronson & Patnoe, 1997). Second, the developmental literature suggests that it is not until adolescence that stereotyped students should regularly experience the stress and underperformance that accompanies explicit evaluations of ability (see Aronson & Good, 2003, for a review). Supporting this prediction, recent stereotype threat research has shown that most children are not meaningfully affected by stereotype threat until the age of 11 or 12 (Good & Aronson, 2003; McKown & Weinstein, 2003). Third, junior high school teachers themselves may unwittingly exacerbate students’ vulnerability to stereotype threat. For example, junior high teachers are more likely than teachers of younger children to believe that their students’ abilities are fixed and less likely to believe that students can increase their abilities through instruction (see Anderman et al.,
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
649
2001). Moreover, as Aronson et al. (2002) argue, anything that promotes a fixed ability mindset exacerbates stereotype threat (also see Aronson, 2002). Consequently, adolescence, particularly the time when students transition to junior high school, may be the most appropriate and effective time to intervene and help students cope with societal stereotypes. Our second goal was to identify interventions that have had particular success in increasing students’ academic outcomes, for both stereotyped and nonstereotyped groups. In designing an effective intervention that would combat vulnerability to stereotype threat, we turned to the social psychology literature. 1.3. Addressing attributions: Effective interventions As discussed previously, stereotype threat disrupts academic performance because the stereotypes provide a pejorative explanation for struggle and difficulty. That is, they raise the possibility—at least in the mind of stereotyped individuals—that the academic difficulties they experience may be due to an internal fault or shortcoming, namely, that they lack the ability to succeed on the task. In a landmark study, Wilson and Linville (1985) designed a clever intervention that addressed precisely these types of pejorative explanations for negative outcomes. Wilson and Linville (1985) argued that these pejorative explanations produce a vicious cycle that leads to and perpetuates poor performance. That is, self-blame for a negative outcome leads to increased anxiety, in turn resulting in poor performance and even more selfblame. Wilson and Linville suggested that to stop the cycle it can help to shift the blame from pejorative attributions (one’s lack of intelligence) to nonpejorative ones (the difficulty of the context), and that this might halt the downward spiral of anxiety and poor performance and ultimately lead to improved academic outcomes. To test this hypothesis, Wilson and Linville convinced entering college students to ‘‘reattribute’’ their academic difficulties from stable internal causes to temporary, external causes. Specifically, they taught the participants in their study that not only do most entering freshmen experience academic difficulties, but that these difficulties lessen after the first year (unstable cause) and are most likely due to the difficulties inherent in transitioning from high school to college (external cause). To make their argument convincing, the participants were exposed to statistics documenting the fact that many first-year college students actually improve their GPAs after the first year. They were also shown videotapes of upperclassmen discussing their college experiences. In these videotapes, the upperclassmen reported that their GPAs were markedly lower in their first year of college. The results were remarkable. The intervention participants not only improved their grades in the second year of college, but also were more likely than nonintervention participants to remain in college. Because of these findings, we hypothesized that changing stereotyped students’ explanations for difficulty from pejorative to nonpejorative can help reduce vulnerability to stereotype threat. In particular, we predicted that stereotyped students who were exposed to a mentoring program that focused on changing attributions for difficulty from pejorative to nonpejorative would outperform students who were exposed to a mentoring program that did not include a focus on attributions.
650
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
In a conceptually similar intervention, Aronson et al. (2002) addressed the implicit beliefs that students have about intelligence in an effort to improve the academic performance of minority college students. Decades of research have shown that students who think of intellectual ability as a fixed trait (entity theory) rather than as a potential that can be developed (incremental theory) are at greater risk of negative academic outcomes—decreased confidence, loss of enjoyment, and performance impairment—when faced with difficulties or setbacks (Dweck & Sorich, 1999; Henderson & Dweck, 1990; Jourden, Bandura, & Banfield, 1991; Martocchio, 1994; Tabernero & Wood, 1999; Wood & Bandura, 1989). Noting that stereotype threat elicits many of the hallmark responses characteristic of entity theorists, Aronson et al. hypothesized that because stereotypes imply fixed, limited ability based on group membership, stereotype threat may temporarily induce an entity-theory mind-set. Consequently, stereotype threat could be overcome by adopting an incremental-theory mind-set (Aronson et al., 2002). In their intervention, Aronson et al. (2002) induced intervention participants to adopt an incremental mind-set by having them watch a highly compelling film depicting the ways the brain changes every time something new is learned. To reinforce this message, the students also participated in an ostensible pen pal program in which they wrote a letter to a struggling junior high student. In their letter, they emphasized the idea that intelligence is expandable and increases with mental work. Results showed that students who had training in the incremental theory reported greater enjoyment of their academic work and greater valuing of academics in general than students in the control group who did not receive the incremental training. In addition, they showed a clear gain in GPA over the other groups. Other interventions in malleability training have resulted in similar academic gains (e.g., Blackwell, Dweck, & Trzesniewski, 2003). Because of these findings, we hypothesized that encouraging stereotyped adolescents to view intelligence as malleable can help reduce vulnerability to stereotype threat. In particular, we predicted that stereotyped students who were exposed to a mentoring program that encouraged an incremental view of intelligence would outperform students who were exposed to a mentoring program that did not include a focus on the malleability of intelligence. 1.4. Goals and hypotheses The results of these interventions for students’ academic achievement are encouraging— they all increase important student school outcomes, such as grades and academic enjoyment. Although grades are certainly important, we wondered whether we could use such techniques to reduce the effects of stereotype threat—and thus raise performance—on standardized tests, which despite continued controversy are becoming more prevalent as markers of learning and merit. Thus, our intervention program tested the efficacy of these past interventions on junior high school students’ performance on the state-administered standardized test. We hypothesized that implementing an intervention that addressed either the pejorative explanations for academic difficulty, or the maladaptive beliefs about the nature of intelligence that stereotypes foster, could reduce the gaps in standardized test performance that typically widen when students enter junior high school (Eccles et al., 1991). Specifically, we predicted that the math performance on the statewide test would be higher for females trained to make nonpejorative
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
651
attributions for difficulty or to appreciate the malleability of intelligence than for females who did not receive this training. We made parallel predictions for the reading test, namely, that all students given either of the two interventions would outperform students in a control group.
2. Method 2.1. Overview In the present study, we investigated whether teaching junior high school students about different perspectives on school achievement could reduce their vulnerability to stereotype threat and increase their standardized test performance. To test our hypotheses, we designed an in-depth intervention in which we taught seventh graders messages we hypothesized would help them cope with stereotype threat. Specifically, we taught seventh-grade students one of two educational messages, or a combination of both messages that we thought would reduce the effects of stereotype threat on their standardized test performance. These messages were conveyed to the seventh graders by college students who mentored the junior high students throughout the school year. All of the students in the study received a college student mentor with whom they discussed various issues, including adjusting to the new school environment and useful study strategies. For one group of students, the mentors also discussed the expandable nature of intelligence and helped students learn more about how the brain is able to form new connections throughout one’s lifetime. For a second group of students, the mentors explained that all students face academic difficulty during the transition to junior high school, but that over time, most students are able to overcome these difficulties and reach high levels of achievement. For a third group of students, the mentors combined these two messages. To reinforce and help students internalize the messages, the students created web pages that advocated, in their own words and pictures, the experimental messages that the students were learning from their mentors. The standardized test performance of these three groups was compared to that of a control group of students who was mentored about the dangers of drug use and who created antidrug web pages. In this way, we were able to determine whether the positive outcomes we were predicting resulted from adopting one (or both) of the two messages about achievement, rather than mere participation in the mentoring program. We predicted that relative to the control group, the participants in the three experimental conditions would receive higher scores on the statewide standardized tests of math and reading, which they took at the end of the school year. Furthermore, we predicted that the benefits of this intervention would occur primarily for the Hispanic students on the reading test, and the female students on the math test. 2.2. Participants and design This study was conducted in a rural school district in Texas that served a largely low-income population, comprised of 63% Hispanic, 15% Black, and 22% White population. Of the Hispanic students, 92% came from homes where the parents spoke English fluently.
652
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
Approximately 70% of these students were qualified to receive reduced cost or free lunches. Of the participants in our study, 67% were Hispanic, 13% were Black, and 20% were White. In addition, 45% of the students were female and 55% were male. Given the research showing stereotype threat effects for students who are Black (Steele & Aronson, 1995), Hispanic (Aronson & Salinas, 1999), female (Spencer et al., 1999), and low-income (Croizet & Claire, 1998), all of the participants in the sample were potentially susceptible to stereotype threat. A total of 138 seventh-grade students (both male and female) who were enrolled in a computer skills class as part of their junior high curriculum participated in the study. Enrollment in the course was randomly determined by the school administration and all students in the course participated in the study. As part of the course curriculum, students learned a variety of computer skills, including e-mailing and web page design. Shortly after the school year began (mid-October), students in the class were randomly assigned a mentor, with whom they communicated in person and via e-mail throughout the school year. Students also were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental conditions that determined which educational message they learned: incremental, attribution, combination, and antidrug conditions. In the incremental condition, students learned about the expandable nature of intelligence; in the attribution condition, students learned about the tendency for all students to initially experience difficulty during seventh grade but then to experience improvement; in the combination condition, students learned about the expandable nature of intelligence and about the temporarily difficult transition to seventh grade; in the antidrug condition (the control condition), students learned about the perils of drug use. At the end of the year, students took statewide standardized tests in math and reading. 2.3. Procedure 2.3.1. Mentors At the beginning of the fall semester (early September) approximately 25 college student mentors from the University of Texas participated in a 3-h training session in which they completed a required mentor-training course designed by the school district. As a supplement to the district’s course, the mentors learned methods of conveying each of the four experimental messages. In order to reduce the possibility of demand characteristics, we told the mentors that we predicted students in all conditions of the experiment to benefit academically from the mentoring program, but the mentors remained blind to the specific hypotheses of the study. To avoid possible effects due to individual differences between mentors, we required each mentor to work with students in each of the four experimental conditions. However, because of scheduling restrictions with the junior high school, this was not possible. Instead, each mentor was randomly assigned mentoring responsibilities for one to two students in three of the four experimental conditions. Thus, each mentor worked with approximately six students. 2.3.2. Computer course As mentioned previously, the participants in the study were seventh-grade students who were enrolled in a computer skills course at the junior high school. At the beginning of the
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
653
school year, students learned basic keyboarding skills such as typing, mouse skills (drag, drop, click, etc.), and Internet navigation. As the students became proficient in these rudimentary skills, they proceeded to more advanced computer topics such as e-mailing and web page design. As a final project in the course, students were required to design their own web page. The computer course instructor told the students that they would each receive a college student mentor who would be available to help them design and build their web page. The instructor further told the students that although the mentors would not be able to come to the class each week, they would be available to answer any questions the students had about their web page or about any problems they were having with school through e-mail. 2.3.3. Intervention procedure After students became proficient in the basics of computer use and e-mailing (midNovember), we began the intervention. The mentors met with their students in person for 90 min in mid-November, and then again for 90 min at the beginning of the second semester (end of January). All remaining communication occurred via the Internet through an e-mail program created specifically for this study. The mentors served three purposes. First, they provided useful advice for the students regarding study skills and any adjustment problems the students may have experienced during the difficult transition to junior high school. Second, they explicitly taught the students one of the four experimental messages: The expandable nature of intelligence (incremental condition), the tendency for all students to initially experience difficulty but then bounce back (attribution condition), a combination of these two messages (combined condition), or the perils of drug use (antidrug control condition). The mentors conveyed these messages in person during the two school visits and through weekly e-mail correspondence with the students throughout the school year. Finally, the mentors helped the students design and create a web page in which the students advocated, in their own words and pictures, the experimental message conveyed by the mentor throughout the year. The mentors told the students that their web pages would serve as public service announcements for other students who were having difficulty in school. Research has repeatedly demonstrated that such advocacies are extremely effective means of getting individuals to adopt the beliefs they are induced to advocate (e.g., Higgins & Rholes, 1978). To further help students internalize the message, and to give them ideas about what to put on their own web page, we designed a ‘‘restricted web space’’ for each of the experimental conditions. In these web spaces, students could ‘‘surf’’ the restricted web and learn in more detail the experimental message. For each condition, access to the restricted web was limited to the condition in which they were assigned. In other words, students in one condition could access the restricted web space that provided information relevant to their condition but not the other conditions. Moreover, while surfing the restricted web, the students could not inadvertently access web pages not affiliated with the study. The mentors encouraged the students in each condition to use the most convincing elements from these restricted web spaces in their own web page.
654
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
2.3.3.1. Incremental condition. Participants in this condition learned that intelligence is not a finite endowment, but rather an expandable capacity that increases with mental work. To reinforce the scientific validity of this perspective, the mentors taught students some facts about the brain and how it works. For example, students learned about the role of neurons and dendrites and how the brain is capable of forming new neural connections throughout one’s life. In addition to hearing this perspective directly from the mentors, students also explored the restricted web space to learn in more detail how the brain works. For example, numerous web pages within the restricted web incorporated animated pictures of the brain, scientific images of neurons and dendrites, and narrative explanations to demonstrate how the brain forms new neural connections when it is engaged in effortful problem solving. Other pages contained various testimonies and catch phrases regarding the expandability of intelligence, such as ‘‘The mind is a muscle; the more you use it, the stronger it grows.’’ 2.3.3.2. Attribution condition. Participants in this condition learned that many students tend to experience difficulty when they move to a new educational situation (such as junior high school) but then bounce back after they become accustomed to their new environment. The mentors explained that in response to academic setbacks, many students erroneously conclude that they are not capable of high academic achievement when, in fact, the difficulties they experience are more likely due to the novelty of the situation. To illustrate, the mentors described their own difficulty in making the adjustment to junior high but that they eventually overcame these difficulties. Furthermore, the mentors pointed out that many aspects of junior high are very different from elementary school, such as changing classes each period, attempting more difficult subjects, meeting many more students, and adjusting to many teachers’ teaching styles rather than just one or two. In short, the mentors encouraged the students to shift their attributions for difficulties from pejorative to nonpejorative causes, that is, from their own shortcomings to the novelty of the situation. As in the incremental condition, students in the attribution condition were encouraged to explore a restricted web space containing information that reinforced this perspective. For example, the students viewed bar graphs showing the average school grades for seventh and eighth graders. The graphs demonstrated that most students earn poor grades in seventh grade—a C average—but that by eighth grade, students perform much better, earning an A average. Other pages contained similar graphs showing that enjoyment of school and school attendance both improve from seventh to eighth grade. Additionally, students viewed other pages that contained testimonials from older students proclaiming that the difficulties of seventh grade get easier over time. 2.3.3.3. Combined condition. Participants in this condition learned both the incremental message and the attribution message. They also explored both restricted web spaces. 2.3.3.4. Antidrug control condition. Participants in this condition learned about the perils of drug use. Specifically, they learned that in addition to the health consequences of using drugs, drugs could also interfere with academic achievement. To reinforce this message, students
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
655
explored a restricted web space to learn about the harmful effects of a variety of drugs, such as amphetamines, cocaine, marijuana, alcohol, and tobacco. 2.3.4. Dependent measure At the end of the school year, we analyzed students’ math and reading achievement as measured by the Texas Assessment of Academic Skills (TAAS) test, a statewide standardized achievement test administered to all students in the district. Many school districts in Texas use the test scores to determine whether a student will be promoted to the next grade or will be retained in the current grade. For example, students who fail to earn a standard score of at least 70 out of a maximum of 100 are often retained in the current grade or are not given course credit for a particular subject. Moreover, it is customary for schools in Texas, as elsewhere, to be evaluated and rewarded as a function of their aggregate test scores. For example, students’ standardized test scores are used to determine if a school is meeting minimum educational requirements for their students. Principals of schools that have a high proportion of students failing to achieve the minimum standard score of 70 are often dismissed. Thus, students, teachers, administrators and parents consider the test to be high stakes, and it is therefore the kind of test likely to produce stereotype threat (Aronson et al., 1999).
3. Results 3.1. Outlier analysis For the math test and the reading test, outliers were identified using the first and third quartiles (Q1 and Q3) and the interquartile range (IQR). Specifically, scores that were less than Q1 1.5(IQR) or greater than Q3 + 1.5(IQR) were not included in the analyses (Iglewicz & Hoaglin, 1993). Because we did not use prior ability as an inclusion criterion for the study, and because some of the students in the study spoke limited English, it was necessary to conduct an outlier analysis. For the analysis of the math scores, this criterion resulted in eliminating five participants’ scores from the 138 participants who took the math test. For the analysis of the reading scores, this criterion resulted in eliminating six participants’ scores from the 135 who took the reading test. It is important to note that the eliminated students did not tend to come from any condition in particular. 3.2. Math achievement test performance To determine if the intervention influenced students’ math standardized test scores, math TAAS scores were submitted to a 2 (gender) 0002 4 (experimental condition) analysis of variance (ANOVA).2 The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect for condition, F(3,125) 2
Without removing the outliers, the ANOVA resulted in a significant main effect of condition, F(3,137) = 2.98, p = .03, qualified by a marginally significant interaction between sex and condition, F(3,137) = 2.52, p = .06.
656
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
= 7.24, p = .001, and a significant main effect for gender, F(1,125) = 4.30, p = .04. These main effects were qualified by a significant gender by condition interaction, F(3,125) = 2.98, p = .03 (see Fig. 1). Planned comparisons indicated that the males in the antidrug condition (M = 81.55, SD = 6.03) performed significantly better on the math test than the females (M = 74.00, SD = 8.37), t(30) = 3.21, p = .002, Cohen’s d = 1.04. However, in all other conditions, the gender gap in math performance disappeared (all ps ns). Importantly, the large effect size between males and females that is present in the control condition was completely erased in all three experimental conditions. Furthermore, although the three experimental manipulations (incremental, attribution, and combined) each increased both male and female students’ math scores as compared to the control condition (antidrug), the manipulations appeared to be particularly beneficial for the female students, as one would expect if their performance were being suppressed by stereotype threat. Compared to the control condition, females achieved significantly higher math scores if they were in the incremental condition (M = 82.11, SD = 5.72), t(26) = 3.34, p = .001,Cohen’s d = 1.13; the attribution condition (M = 84.53, SD = 5.41), t(25) = 4.29, p = .001, Cohen’s d = 1.50; or the combined condition (M = 84.06, SD = 7.09), t(26) = 4.14, p = .001, Cohen’s d = 1.30. Again, these are all large effect sizes, indicating that the intervention procedures meaningfully increased females’ math scores compared to the control condition. The only marginally significant differences on math scores for male students in the four conditions occurred between the control (antidrug) condition (M = 81.55, SD = 6.03) and the incremental condition (M = 85.25, SD = 5.42), t(40) = 1.95, p = .054, Cohen’s d = .64 (all other ps ns). 3.3. Reading achievement test scores To determine if the intervention influenced students’ reading test scores, reading TAAS scores were submitted to a one-way ANOVA comparing performance of students in the four
Fig. 1. Average math scores on the TAAS test.
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
657
Fig. 2. Average reading scores on the TAAS test.
experimental conditions.3 Although we were interested in comparing reading achievement for minority and White students, the sample did not contain enough White students to perform these comparisons. The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of condition, F(3,125) = 2.71, p = .05 (see Fig. 2). Planned comparisons indicated that students in the incremental condition (M = 88.26, SD = 7.17) and students in the Attribution condition (M = 89.62, SD = 7.01) achieved significantly higher scores on the reading test than students in the control condition (M = 84.38, SD = 7.79), t(65) = 2.07, p = .041, Cohen’s d = .52; and t(61) = 2.72, p = .008, Cohen’s d = .71, respectively. There were no differences between the combined condition (M = 86.71, SD = 8.70) and the other conditions.
4. Discussion Findings were consistent with hypotheses for the most part. Results showed that the typical gender gap in math standardized test performance emerged for the participants in the control condition. That is, boys outperformed girls on the math test if they had been mentored in the harmful consequences of drug use. However, when the participants learned about the expandability of intelligence the gender gap in math performance disappeared. The incremental condition increased both boys’ and girls’ math performance, but this increase in math scores was particularly pronounced for the female students, which is consistent with predictions derived from analysis of stereotype threat processes. Similarly, the gender gap in math performance disappeared when participants were encouraged to make nonpejorative attributions for their difficulties and when they were exposed to both the incremental and the reattribution intervention message. 3
Without removing the outliers, the ANOVA did not result in a significant main effect of condition. Post hoc analyses, however, confirmed that students in the incremental and attribution conditions scored significantly higher on the reading test than did the students in the control condition.
658
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
As with math performance, a similar pattern of increased achievement was found on the reading test. Students who were mentored in the malleability of intelligence performed better on the reading test than students who were mentored in the perils of drug use. Because the participants in the study were primarily from low socioeconomic backgrounds and 80% of the participants were either Black or Hispanic, all were potentially vulnerable to experiencing stereotype threat (Aronson & Salinas, 1999; Croizet & Claire, 1998; Steele & Aronson, 1995). Consequently, encouraging adolescents to make nonpejorative explanations for difficulty—that is, to think of intelligence as expandable rather than fixed or to attribute difficulties to the novelty of the situation rather than their own shortcomings—can meaningfully increase student achievement, especially for those students who face negative stereotypes about their abilities such as Hispanic, Black, low-income, and female students. The results of this intervention were encouraging. Stereotyped students—females in math, and ability-stigmatized students in reading—increased their standardized test scores after participating in the intervention program. Initially, we hoped to determine which of the intervention messages—the incremental nature of intelligence or the reattribution training— would have the most beneficial effects. Furthermore, we had originally hypothesized that combining the incremental message with the reattribution message would increase students’ standardized test scores more than either message alone. Interestingly, we found that both messages increased students’ standardized test scores. Combining the two messages did not appear to have an additive effect, which was surprising since the two intervention messages were, at least superficially, distinct. In hindsight, however, the lack of an additive effect is understandable. Although the two intervention messages implemented in this intervention appear different on the surface, they are at heart, very similar. Importantly, closer inspection of Dweck’s work on implicit theories of intelligence reveals a clear kinship to attribution theory. As Dweck and her colleagues repeatedly have shown, the attributions one makes for poor performance depend upon one’s beliefs about the nature of intelligence (see Dweck, 1999, for a review). This research has shown, for example, that entity theorists are more likely to blame their own shortcomings for academic difficulties or failures. That is, they make internal, stable attributions for negative outcomes. Not coincidently, these are precisely the kinds of pejorative attributions that attribution theory predicts can lead to the downward spiral of self-blame, anxiety, and underperformance. Alternatively, incremental theorists are much less likely to blame their own intellectual shortcomings for their current struggles. Instead, incremental theorists view mistakes as an indicator that they did not try hard enough or did not approach the problem appropriately. In other words, they make external, unstable attributions for negative outcomes. Clearly, encouraging students to view intelligence as expandable does not simply change their beliefs about intelligence; more importantly, it also changes the attributions they make for the causes of their difficulties. Given the underlying similarities of the constructs, it is not surprising, then, that both techniques led to higher test scores and did not produce additive effects when both were presented in combination. It is quite plausible that both interventions simply addressed the same underlying concerns. Regardless of the specific message that led to the improvements, we note the ease with which our intervention led to significant increases in students’ standardized test scores.
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
659
Student performance did not improve through additional skills drilling or cramming of content related to the test. Rather, students improved by learning attitudes that helped them contend with the anxieties that research has shown develop in part from their social identities. This, we believe, is important not only as validation of a theoretically derived psychological intervention, but also as grounds for further questioning the use of standardized tests as markers of ability and learning. Moreover, this adds a somewhat discouraging note to our findings. Standardized test scores may be poor predictors of a student’s future academic success (e.g., Jencks & Phillips, 1998), yet they are nevertheless used as indicators of a student’s current and future abilities. Students with high SAT scores have a much better chance of being admitted to the college of their choice. Similarly, statewide standardized test scores in elementary and junior high school are no longer simply used as indicators of a school’s effectiveness; they increasingly are used to determine whether or not a student may proceed to the next grade, earn credit in a required course, and ultimately, graduate from high school. Our intervention significantly boosted the performance of girls, minority, and low-income students by addressing the psychologically threatening nature of these assessments. Hence, our results provide further evidence of the fragility of the standard measures used to assess learning, potential, and ultimately, success. Despite this sobering note, it is gratifying to see performance rise in response to a psychological intervention. Indeed, widespread endorsement of the belief in malleable intelligence and nonpejorative attributions for difficulties may render standardized testing more equitable for students who must contend with stereotypes impugning their intellectual abilities. Based on the previous discussion one might wonder whether our intervention succeeded because it directly reduced or eliminated stereotype threat altogether. We believe that despite the success of our intervention, stereotype threat remains a potential obstacle for students to overcome. As Aronson et al. (2002) demonstrated, encouraging students to adopt an incremental view of intelligence does not alleviate perceptions of a stereotype-threatening environment. In their study, African American participants reported greater feelings of being judged by others through the lens of stereotypes than did White students, regardless of experimental condition. Thus, as in the Aronson et al. study, our intervention likely succeeded by changing stereotyped students’ responses to a stereotype threatening situation rather than changing their direct perception of stereotype threat. Clearly, many junior high students—particularly Black, Hispanic, low-income, and female students—may experience the intellectual emasculation that Dr. de Grasse Tyson felt during much of his academic life. The current research is encouraging because it demonstrates a successful strategy for stemming the spiral of self-blame, anxiety, and underperformance that many adolescents experience.
Acknowledgements This research was supported by a William T. Grant Scholars award and a grant from the Russel Sage Foundation to Joshua Aronson, and by a Spencer Foundation/American
660
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
Educational Research Association fellowship awarded to Catherine Good. We also thank the Center for Research on Culture, Development, and Education for supporting this work. We would like to thank Jeannetta Williams and David Disko for their assistance in conducting this research. We also thank Karen Rester for her helpful comments on an earlier draft.
References Anderman, E. M., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Roeser, R., Wigfield, A., & Blumenfeld, P. (2001). Learning to value mathematics and reading: Relations to mastery and performance-oriented instructional practices. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 76 – 95. Aronson, E., & Patnoe, S. (1997). The jigsaw classroom. New York: Longman. Aronson, J. (2002). Stereotype threat: Contending and coping with unnerving expectations. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on education ( pp. 279 – 301). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Aronson, J., Fried, C., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113 – 125. Aronson, J., & Good, C. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype vulnerability in adolescents. In F. Pajares, & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education: Vol. 2. Academic motivation of adolescents ( pp. 299 – 330). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When White men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29 – 46. Aronson, J., Quinn, D., & Spencer, S. (1998). Stereotype threat and the academic performance of minorities and women. In J. Swim, & C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The target’s perspective ( pp. 83 – 103). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Aronson, J., & Salinas, M. (1999). Stereotype threat: The role of effort withdrawal and apprehension on the intellectual underperformance of Mexican-Americans. Unpublished manuscript. Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (in press). Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Blackwell, L. S., Dweck, C. S., & Trzesniewski, K. (2003). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. (Unpublished manuscript). New York: Columbia University. Blascovich, J., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D., & Steele, C. (2001). African Americans and high blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat. Psychological Science, 12, 225 – 229. The College Board (2002). 2002 College bound seniors: A profile of SAT program test takers [On-line]. Available: http://www.collegeboard.com/prod_downloads/about/news_info/cbsenior/yr2002/pdf/2002_TOTAL_ GROUP_REPORT.pdf. Retrieved May 20, 2003. Croizet, J., & Claire, T. (1998). Extending the concept of stereotype and threat to social class: The intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 588 – 594. de Grasse Tyson, N. (1991). PhD convocation address: St Paul’s Chapel, Columbia University [On-line]. Available: http://research.amnh.org/users/tyson/speeches/PhDConvocationAddress.html. Retrieved January 25, 2003. Dweck, C. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Dweck, C. S., & Sorich, L. (1999). Mastery-oriented thinking. In C. R. Snyder (Ed.), Coping ( pp. 232 – 251). New York: Oxford University Press.
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
661
Eccles, J. S., & Jacobs, J. E. (1992). The impact of mothers’ gender-role stereotypic beliefs on mothers’ and children’s ability perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 932 – 944. Eccles, J. S., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we doing to early adolescents? The impact of educational contexts on early adolescents. American Journal of Education, 99, 521 – 542. Expanding Your Horizons. Available: http://www.expandingyourhorizons.org/. Retrieved April 16, 2003. Felner, R. D., Primavera, J., & Cauce, A. M. (1981). The impact of school transitions: A focus for preventive efforts. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9, 449 – 459. Good, C., & Aronson, J. (2003). The development of stereotype threat and its relation to theories of intelligence: Effects on elementary school girls’ mathematics achievement and task choices (Unpublished manuscript). New York: Columbia University. Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). The effects of a stereotype threatening environment on women’s sense of belonging to mathematics (Unpublished raw data). New York: Columbia University. Good, C., Aronson, J., & Harder, J. A. (1999). Stereotype threat in the absence of a kernel of truth: Women in Calculus. (Unpublished manuscript). Austin: The University of Texas. Henderson, V., & Dweck, C. S. (1990). Achievement and motivation in adolescence: A new model and data. In S. Feldman, & G. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent ( pp. 308 – 329). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. S. (1978). Saying is believing: Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 363 – 378. Iglewicz, B., & Hoaglin, D. C. (1993). How to detect and handle outliers. Milwaukee, WI: American Society for Quality Control. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychological Science, 11, 365 – 371. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (in press). Do high-achieving female students underperform in private? The implications of threatening environments on intellectual processing. Journal of Educational Psychology. Jencks, C., & Phillips, M. (1998). The Black – White test score gap. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. Jourden, F., Bandura, A., & Banfield, J. (1991). The impact of conceptions of ability on self-regulatory factors and motor skill acquisition. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 213 – 226. Martocchio, J. J. (1994). Effects of concepts of ability on anxiety, self-efficacy, and learning in training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 819 – 825. McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype-consciousness in childhood. Child Development, 74, 498 – 515. National Center for Education Statistics (2003). The Condition of Education (Publication No. 2003-067). Available: NCES Web site http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2003/2003067.pdf. Retrieved June 20, 2003. Romo, H., & Falbo, T. (1995). Latino high school graduation: Defying the odds. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1994). Failing at fairness: How our schools cheat girls. New York: Touchstone. Simmons, R. G., Black, A., & Zhou, Y. (1991). African-American versus white children and the transition to junior high school. American Journal of Education, 99, 481 – 520. Spencer, S., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4 – 28. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613 – 629. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797 – 811. Steele, C. M., Spencer, S., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology ( pp. 379 – 440). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Tabernero, C., & Wood, R. E. (1999). Implicit theories versus the social construal of ability in self-regulation and performance on a complex task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 104 – 127.
662
C. Good et al. / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 645–662
Valencia, R. R. (1997). Latinos and education: An overview of sociodemographic characteristics and schooling conditions and outcomes. In M. Barrera-Yepes (Ed.), Latino education issues: Conference proceedings ( pp. 13 – 37). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Wilson, T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1985). Improving the performance of college freshmen with attributional techniques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 287 – 293. Wood, R., & Bandura, A. (1989). Impact of conceptions of ability on self-regulatory mechanisms and complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 407 – 415.